Thanks to IFERI committee member, Dr Molly de Lemos, for drawing attention to this excellent piece by Laura Stewart following on from the Dehaene lecture
.
Laura Stewart's piece is extremely readable and informed by evidence - and another 'must' read to appreciate the importance of explicit teaching of phonics which should include providing learners with 'matched' texts to apply their phonics knowledge and decoding skill rather than predictable texts which bring about poor and potentially damaging reading habits/profiles.
What We Know About Beginning Reading
Laura D. Stewart
Vice President, Professional Development–Superkids
Zaner-Bloser
Research consistently shows that strong readers effortlessly recognize words and link words to their meanings in order to comprehend text. Skilled reading happens so quickly and efficiently that it seems as though words are recognized as whole units. But the evidence is clear that readers do attend to the inner structure of words (Adams, 1990; Ehri, 2005), assembling the letters and sounds very quickly—in as little as 150 milliseconds for a word (Shaywitz, 2003). We know that the “immediacy of reading is an illusion based on the extreme automaticity of these word-assembly steps, which operate outside our conscious awareness” (Dahaene, 2009).
Learning to decode words is the critical step in beginning reading because it requires students to process the inner structure of words. Students become familiar with common letter patterns through decoding, gradually increasing the speed with which they recognize words and letter sequences. This leads to automatic and effortless word recognition, which then allows the reader to focus cognitive energy on comprehension rather than word recognition. When the brain is freed up from the confines of decoding, it can focus on the real purpose for reading—comprehension.
The role of instruction
To build the neural connections necessary for decoding, readers need to be taught the relationship between letters and sounds—phonics—explicitly and systematically. A large body of evidence confirms that systematic, explicit phonics is the most critical component of beginning reading instruction (Adams, 1990; McCardle and Chhabra, 2004; Christensen and Bowey, 2005; Ehri et al., 2001; Foorman et al., 1998; Foorman et al., 2001; Moats, 1998; NICHD, 2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998, Rayner et al., 2001).
What does systematic, explicit instruction mean? Systematic instruction is characterized by the use of a method or system. The skills that are needed to acquire the alphabetic principle should not be learned opportunistically. To be effective, instruction must be organized into a logical sequence (Armbruster, et al., 2001). Explicit instruction is clearly, intentionally stated and not merely implied. It is direct instruction that does not make assumptions about the skills and knowledge that children acquire on their own. Explicit phonics instruction teaches the sound/symbol relationships before children meet them in words (Dixon, et al., 1992).
For children to develop the decoding habit, they must receive extensive instruction in phonics with ample opportunities to practice their decoding in connected text. Decoding skills are best practiced with text that matches the letter-sounds as children are learning them. This kind of text is called phonetically controlled, or decodable, text.
The importance of appropriate text in beginning reading
“The types of words which appear in beginning reading texts may well exert a more powerful influence in shaping children’s word identification strategies than the method of reading instruction.” (Juel and Roper-Schneider, 1985)
The text that teachers select impacts the kind of word identification strategies that students will use in their reading. So primary teachers need to think: What are my goals for my beginning readers? How do I develop sustainable strategies for getting the text off the page?
Using uncontrolled text in beginning reading: When beginning readers are given text for which they do not know the phonetic code, they develop an over-reliance on context and pictures. While context and pictures are useful for confirming meaning, they are not designed for initial word recognition. Students who develop context and pictures as their first word recognition strategies are essentially guessing, which puts them at a tremendous risk for reading failure, as they have not developed the decoding accuracy or automaticity necessary for reading success.
Using text based on “sight words”: Students who read from texts that are primarily constructed around high-frequency words will primarily employ a visual strategy for word identification, a strategy that will not sustain a reader very far into the process of reading (Foorman, et al., 2004), as children cannot begin to memorize all the words necessary to read.
Using predictable or pattern texts: Students who read from these texts, common in early “leveled readers,” will memorize patterns, repetitive language, and rhyme, but they are unlikely to acquire strong phonetic decoding skills. Students using patterns will not have developed a sustainable strategy for unlocking text. In fact, studies have shown that students in programs that emphasize these types of text fare poorly when compared to students in programs which employ decodable text (Foorman, et al., 1998).
Using decodable text:
Students who read from decodable text will develop a phonetic decoding strategy based on letter-sound correspondences. This is the best and most reliable strategy to develop in beginning readers, as this matches what brain science tells us about the need to establish a neural connection between sounds and letters (Shaywitz and Shaywitz, 2004). Decodable text allows students to get reliable results when the meet new words and reinforces their mastery of and automatic reliance on decoding. Furthermore, to become proficient readers, children must read great amounts of connected text (Beck & Juel, 1995; Snow, et al.,) but in the beginning are challenged by their struggle to identify words. Decodable text supports beginning readers so that they may have considerable successful practice with connected text. The combination of the accurate and automatic decoding and the practice with connected text will equip readers as they encounter higher levels of text with multi-syllabic and unknown words.
Although decodable text is important, the use of any decodable text alone is not sufficient for students to develop phonetic decoding skills. The text must match the instruction. In review of the research on decodable text, Mesmer (2001) notes that researchers have defined decodability by the presence of two primary features, 1) a proportion of words with phonetically regular relationships between letters and sounds, and 2) a degree of match between the letter/sound relationships represented in text and those that the reader has been taught. This match between instruction and application is critical; decodable text serves as a “ conduit for the application of phonics instruction” (Mesmer, 2001; p. 130).
Research on decodable text and student achievement
In a recent review of the empirical evidence regarding the influence of decodable text on reading performance and growth, Cheatham and Allor (2012) examined seven high-quality peer-reviewed studies (Compton et al, 2004; Hiebert and Fisher, 2007; Hoffman et al., 2001; Mesmer, 2010; Jenkins et al., 2004; Juel and Roper/Schneider, 1985; Mesmer, 2005) and concluded that “decodability is a critical characteristic of early reading text as it increases the likelihood that students will use a decoding strategy and results in immediate benefits, particularly in regard to accuracy” (p. 2241). They further state: “Theoretical research and empirical evidence support the need for students to apply phonics skills in connected text” and “Evidence is very clear that decodable text positively impacts early reading progress” (p. 2242).
In conclusion
The beginning years of learning to read are extraordinary! Children are mastering a complex system of sounds and symbols, and learning to derive meaning from print. The goal of effective primary-grade reading instruction is not only to teach children how to read, but to foster a love of reading in every child. Children who experience early reading success want to read more. This success leads to confident, independent, and engaged readers—readers who are able to tackle the demands of higher-order comprehension of complex text.
Bibliography
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning About Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Armbruster, B.B., Lehr, F., & Osborn, J. M. (2001). Put Reading First: The research building blocks of teaching children to read. Kindergarten through grade 3. Washington DC: National Institute for Literacy.
Beck, I. L., & Juel, C. (1995). The role of decoding in learning to read. The American Educator, 8, 21-25, 39-42
Cheatham, J.P. & Allor, J. H. (2012). “The influence of decodability in early reading text on reading achievement: A review of the evidence.” Reading and Writing 25, no. 9.
Chall, J. S. (1983) Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Christensen, C. A., & Bowey, J. A. (2005). “The efficacy of orthographic rime, grapheme-phoneme correspondence, and implicit phonics approaches to teaching decoding skills.” Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, no. 4, pages 327–349.
Compton, D.L, Appleton, A.C., & Hosp, M.K. (2004). Exploring the relationship between text-leveling systems and reading accuracy and fluency in second-grade students who are average and poor readers. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 19, 176-184.
Dahaene, S. (2009.) Reading in the Brain: The Science and Evolution of a Human Invention. New York: Penguin Group
Dixon, R., Carnine, D., & Kame’enui, E. (1992). Curriculum Guidelines for Diverse Learners. Eugene: University of Oregon, National Center to Improve the Tools of Educators.
Ehri, L. C. (2002). “Phases of acquisition in learning to read words and implications for teaching.” In Learning and Teaching Reading, edited by R. Stainthorp and P. Tomlinson. London: British Journal of Educational Psychology Monograph Series II.
Ehri, L. C. (2005). “Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues.” Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, pages 167-188.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S.A., & Willows, D. M. (2001). “Systematic phonics instruction helps students learn to read: Evidence from the national reading panel’s meta-analysis.” Review of Educational Research 71, no. 3, pages 393–447.
Ehri, L.C. & Snowling, M. (2004). Developmental variation in word recognition. In A. C. Stone, E. R. Silliman, B. J. Ehren, & K Apel (Eds), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders (pp. 443-460). New York: Guilford.
Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998). “The role of instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk children.” Journal of Educational Psychology 90, no. 1, pages 37–55.
Foorman, B. R., & Torgesen, J. K. (2001). “Critical elements of classroom and small-group instruction promote reading success in all children.” Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice 16, no. 4, pages 203–212.
Foorman, B.R., Francis, D. J., Davidson, K., Harm, K., & Griffin, J. (2004). “Variability in text features in six grade 1 basal reading programs,” Scientific Studies of Reading 8, no. 2, pages 167–197.
Hiebert, E. H., & Fisher, C. W. (2007). Critical word factor in texts for beginning readers. The Journal of Educational Research, 101, 3-11.
Hoffman, J.V., Roser, N.L., Patterson, E.U., Salas, R., & Pennington, J. (2001). Text leveling and “little books” in first grade reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 33, 507-528.
Jenkins, J.R., Vadasy, P. F., Peyton, J. A., & Sanders, E. A. (2003). “Decodable text—where to find it.” Reading Teacher, 57, no. 2, 185-189.
Jenkins, J.R., Peyton, J. A., Sanders, E. A., & Vadasy, P. F. (2004). “Effects of reading
decodable texts in supplemental first-grade tutoring,” Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, no. 1, pages 53-86.
Juel, C. & Roper/Schneider, D. (1985). “The influence of basal readers on first grade reading,” Reading Research Quarterly 20, no. 2, pages 134–152.
McCardle, P. & Chhabra, V., eds. (2004). The Voice of Evidence in Reading Research. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
Mesmer, H. A. E. (2001). Decodable text: A review of what we know. Reading Research and Instruction, 40, 121-142.
Mesmer, H. A. E. (2005). Text decodability and the first-grade reader. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 21, 61-86.
Mesmer, H. A. E. (2010). Textual scaffolds for developing fluency in beginning readers: Accuracy and reading rate in qualitively leveled and decodable text. Literacy Research and Instruction, 49, 20-39.
Moats, L. C. (1998). “Teaching Decoding.” American Educator 22, no. 1–2 , pages 42–49, 95–96.
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common core state standards for English/language arts & literacy in history/social studies, science and technical subjects. Washington DC: Author.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading Instruction. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Peretsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). “How psychological science informs the teaching of reading.” Psychological Science in the Public Interest 2, no. 2, pages 31–74.
Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming Dyslexia. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Shaywitz, S., & Shaywitz, B. (2004) “Reading disability and the brain,” Educational Leadership, 61, no. 6, pages 6–11.
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P., eds. (1998)s Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council.
Vadasy, P.F., Sanders, E. A., & Peyton, J. A. (2005) “Relative effectiveness of reading
practice or word-level instruction in supplemental tutoring: How text matters,” Journal of Learning Disabilities 38, no. 4, pages 364-380.